Textual typology and transcription problems: analysis of the data

In this section, the results obtained by the program are analyzed. The analysis procedure has always been the same for each of the linguistic phenomena studied:

First, the relation between frequency of occurrence of

the features and textual typology is stated.

Second, we evaluated whether this relation confirms the original hypothesis, which states that certain kinds of texts are harder to transcribe, according to the scales of difficulty.

Number of dialogic turns

The first feature to be analyzed is the number of dialogic turns, understood as the number of times a speaker replaces another in the conversation. According to the original hypothesis, in the analysis of the data it is assumed that there is a direct relation between the number of turns and the effort needed in the transcription process.

Below, it is shown how this feature is reflected in the mentioned classifications from a quantitative point of view.

In Figure 1, which analyses the degree of formality (scale 1), it can be observed how the participants in informal texts produce shorter turns, while those turns belonging to formal texts are longer and those turns produced in media texts have an intermediate length, closer to formal texts than to informal ones.

In the groups dealing with number of speakers (Scale 2), apart from the obvious conclusion about monologues, those turns belonging to dialogues are almost two and a half words longer than those belonging to conversations.

This result confirms the original hypothesis, that is to say: the higher the number of speakers, the shorter are the turns (considering the number of words per turn) and therefore the bigger is the effort necessary in the transcription. Furthermore, it has been proved that shorter turns are typical of informal texts and so it is in this area of the corpus where the transcriber will find more difficulties.

Overlapping

This second feature is directly related to the previous one and represents, according to C-ORAL-ROM transcribers, one of the most important difficulties in the transcription task: overlapping. Again, the results are obtained dividing the number of words by the number of overlapping cases (except in monologues, where there is obviously no overlapping).Figure 3 is the confirmation of Figure 1. As expected, overlapping is less frequent in the formal and media genres than in the informal one. In the informal genre, as shown in Figure 4, the difference between dialogues and conversations is an average of almost ten words. These data prove that overlapping is prototypical of the informal genre and, furthermore, of the conversation subgenre. As far as the transcription task is concerned, this fact puts the conversation subgenre on the furthest end in terms of transcription difficulty.

Speaking speed

Another important feature for the transcriber is the speed at which the participants speak. These are the results obtained for C-ORAL-ROM.

This feature, expressed in words per second, confirms once more how, in terms of speaking speed and given that the faster a participant speaks the harder the is to transcribe, the informal genre and the conversational subgenre are the most laborious in the transcription task. As we can see in the figures, a prototypical participant in an informal conversation utters approximately three and a half words per second, while for formal texts and informal monologues the average is 2.6-2.7 words per second.

Fragmented words

So far, the data has confirmed the original hypothesis. However, in regards to the frequency of fragmented words, the hypothesis was not supported. A fragmented word occurs when a speaker does not complete the utterance of the word.

In Figure 7, it becomes obvious that most participants in the media genre are speaking professionals. Even though they speak at a higher speed than those appearing in formal texts (Figure 5), the frequency of occurrence of fragmented words in this kind of texts is much lower than it is in other genres, which share almost the same ratio. On the other hand, the formal genre is characterized by a high number of fragmented words.

Also, unexpectedly, Figure 8 shows that the number of fragmented words is higher in dialogues than it is in monologues and conversations. The fact that dialogues are not in an intermediate position (as it happens in the rest of the results) leads to the conclusion that there is not a direct relation between number of participants and frequency of occurrence of fragmented words, an hypothesis that should be confirmed with further data.

All this would show how, in the transcription process, fragmented words are not perceived by the transcriber as an added difficulty, given that, in the difficulty scale (Scale 1 ), the formal genre is the easiest to transcribe.

Supports

The following analysis corresponds to the frequency of occurrence of supports

Figure 9 shows an opposite arrangement to the difficulty order originally suggested, where formal texts were classified as the easiest ones. Similarly to the case of fragmented words, the prototypical participant in a formal recording resorts to supports every 46 words, in order to sustain his discourse. This contrasts with the ratio in informal texts, where the average is of almost 96 words.

The information presented in this figure is quite

unexpected, especially when, in this sense, the formal genre is made up of communicative interactions such as conferences or lessons in an academic context, which are quite close to texts following some kind of script. This helps to understand this phenomenon not as a symptom of lack of planning, but as a support which participants in this kind of recordings find useful or necessary.

These data could somehow be connected to the number of participants, that is to say, the more the speakers in a conversation, the less supports are used, due to the dynamics of the interaction. In order to confirm this hypothesis, we can look at Figure 10, where the number of participants is one of the parameters.

Nevertheless, this table shows how conversations represent the texts with the lowest frequency of occurrence of supports. In fact, the data prove that, as the number of participants increases (and, if we look at Figure 2, the turn is longer), so do the frequencies of supports.

Therefore, this is again a revealing finding. First of all, for the description of the different linguistic registers of the spoken corpus and further studies on this field. Secondly, it is also important for the transcribers, as supports do not seem to constitute an added difficulty in the transcription process.

Retractings

Finally, figures relating the frequency of occurrence of retractings were analyzed in the six groups, obtaining the following results.

Again, the scale of difficulty is inverted. Even though the frequency of occurrence of retractings increases in the informal texts, and this matches the predictions made, it is interesting to observe how the formal and media genres invert their positions with respect to the figures. This leads to important conclusions regarding the differences between these two genres, which are in principle quite similar, given that both are planned and are characterized by a register which is close to written language.

As for the results in Figure 12, retracting is a characteristic  phenomenon  of  informal  monologues, which again raises the question of the motive behind this phenomenon. It is interesting to remark that this feature presents the highest frequencies in a kind of text where there is no interaction at all between participants.

Regarding the transcription problems, again the original hypothesis is inverted. Contrary to what was expected, there is not a direct relation between the presence of retractings and the degree of difficulty in the transcription process, as the informal monologue is the last in the scale of difficulty (Scale 2) introduced in section 3.

Conclusions

Sometimes, the obvious facts has to be proven in order to question its value of truth, and this is exactly what has been accomplished in this paper. Starting from a apparently natural hypothesis, which consists in relating the presence of certain spoken features to a special difficulty in the transcription process, it has been deduced from the analysis of the results that this hypothesis is not always true because there are some spoken features such as supports, whose frequency of occurrence is higher in those texts which, as it is the case with formal texts and informal monologues, are not an added obstacle for the transcriber.

All this would lead to the classification of the features of the corpus into two groups:

  • Interactional features: number of words per turn, frequency of overlapping and speaking speed (Figures 1-6 ).
  • Production features: frequency of occurrence of fragmented words, supports and retractings (Figures 7-12).

The distribution of the types of text in the case of group 1 matches exactly the intuitive difficulty scales presented as scales 1 and 2 .

However, the cases in group 2 (production features) show a distribution which is even opposite to scales 1 and 2 in some of its aspects: media is the genre with less influence coming from fragmented words and retractings, and the formal genre is the one with a highest ratio of fragmented words and supports (this last feature shows its lowest ratio in informal texts).

As for the second scale (informal texts), observing the second group of features (production features), conversations appear as the less affected subgenre, while monologues stand out as the richest in supports and retractings. The scales, if only the production features were taken into account, would be as the following, from less to most difficult:

  • difficult—————————– + difficult

media      informal     formal

Figure 2: Scale 1 and production features.

  • difficult—————————– + difficult

conversations dialogues monologues Figure 3: Scale 2 and production features.

The fact that interactional features match exactly the intuitions made at the beginning and that production features are almost the opposite (the highest difficulty end corresponds to formal texts and informal monologues)

gives way to the conclusion that the difficulty perceived by the transcriber comes from the features in the first group (interactional). This is shown below in the correlation of the data for interactional features in Scale 1 (that is, the data on figures 1 to 6 for informal, formal and media). It is clearly shown on the figures how the order informal-media-formal is kept at all the times. The three figures correspond to relations between speed and overlapping (Figure 13), speed and words per turn (Figure 14) and overlapping and words per turn (Figure 15).

Those features belonging to the second group (production features) are problematic in the establishment of the text (Benveniste, 1998) phase, as it has to be decided what is going to be the written representation for that kind of recording; however, the transcriber does not consider these features as obstacles in the transcription process.

These empirical conclusions should be confirmed by applying the same analysis on new texts, as it will be the case with a previously created corpus in LLI- UAM: CORLEC (Moreno, 2002). CORLEC does not follow exactly the same transcription criteria as C-ORAL-ROM

ROM (mainly because it was recorded and transcribed 10 years before), but it has the advantage of being three times larger in terms of the number of words.

Nonetheless,   the   main   conclusion   (the complexity of a transcription derives from the interaction features and not from the production features) is fully justified by the representative character of the data used as a basis. More specifically, there were 429 different speakers, some of them participating in different recordings, which means a total of 554 participants. The number of texts is not very high (169 recordings) but its great variety should be highlighted (as shown in the distribution chapter). Finally, below is a summary of the data used:

Total data in C-ORAL-ROM (general data)
TextsSpeakersParticipantsTurnsWords
16942955415595312597
Total data in C-ORAL-ROM (features)
OverlappingRetractingsFragmented wordsSupports
4307786030844807

Table 16: Absolute data for C-ORAL-ROM.

Further investigation applying this methodology, extended to other criteria, might include characterizing the different spoken registers included in C-ORAL-ROM at all the linguistic levels. Besides, an optimum result of applying this methodology would lead to a prediction of the typology of a given spoken text, based on quantitative data not in qualitative ones.

References

4.   References.

Blanche-Benveniste, C. (1998). Estudios lingüísticos sobre la relación entre oralidad y escritura. Barcelona, Gedisa.

Briz, A. (1996). El español coloquial: situación y uso. Madrid, Arco Libros.

Cresti, E. et al. (2002). The C-ORAL-ROM project. New methods for spoken language archives in a multilingual romance corpus. In Proceedings of LREC 2002. Las Palmas de Gran Canaria.

Gallardo, B (1993). La transición entre turnos conversacionales: silencios, solapamientos e interrupciones. In Contextos, XI/21-22, (pp. 189–220).

Halliday, M.A.K. (1985). Spoken and Written Language. Oxford University Press.

Listerri,       J.      (1997).      Transcripción,      etiquetado     y codificación de    corpus orales. In Fundación Duques de Soria. Seminario de Industrias de la Lengua. http://liceu.uab.es/~joaquim/publicacions/FDS97.html

Add Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

error: Este contenido está sometido a copyright.